Motor Abilities in Infants at Risk for Autism


Lloyd, M., MacDonald, M., & Lord, C. (2013). Motor Skills of Toddlers with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Autism : The International Journal of Research and Practice, 17(2), 133–146. 

With increased interest in the early diagnosis and treatment of children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD), more attention has been called to the motor skills of very young children with ASD. This study describes the gross and fine motor skills of a cross-sectional group of 162 children with ASD between the ages of 12 and 36 months, as well as a subset of 58 children followed longitudinally. Gross motor and fine motor age equivalent scores were obtained for all children. A 'motor difference' variable was calculated for each child's gross and fine motor skills by taking the absolute difference of the children's age equivalent motor score and their respective chronological age. In Study 1 (the cross-sectional analysis), ANCOVA (co-varied for nonverbal problem solving) revealed significant group differences in the gross motor and fine motor age difference variables. Post-hoc analysis revealed that gross motor and fine motor differences became significantly greater with each 6-month period of chronological age. In Study 2, 58 children were measured twice, an average of 12 months apart. Results indicate that the gross motor and fine motor difference scores significantly increased between the first and second measurements. The importance of addressing motor development in early intervention treatments is discussed.

Extract

Active play facilitates the development of motor skills, social skills, an understanding of the world, daily living skills, and adaptive behaviour; it also provides a unique opportunity for young children to be physically active and play games with peers (Pellegrini & Smith, 1998;Ridgers, Stratton, & Fairclough, 2006; Sutera et al., 2007).

Children with ASD, by definition, have difficulties in the social domain, thereby limiting their time engaged in play, with or without other children (Dewey, Lord, & Magill, 1988).

The children with ASD in this study achieved their motor milestones within typical ranges which is similar to the findings of other studies (Dawson, Osterling, Meltzoff, & Kuhl, 2000; Ozonoff, et al., 2008). However as the toddlers with ASD got older their fundamental motor skills fell significantly behind what would be expected for their chronological age.

Fundamental gross motor skills are complex and require coordination, motor planning and control. These skills are often learned through imitation in social contexts. We hypothesize that the slowing of gross motor development found in this study is partly due to the fact that gross motor skills are commonly explored and discovered during self-directed or self-regulated learning both with and without peers during play. This type of play and social engagement is a challenge for children with ASD. It is also possible that tactile sensitivities and/or aversions could contribute to the delays in fundamental motor skills. Therefore we propose that the limitations in motor proficiency demonstrated by the toddlers in this study may create a cycle where poor motor skills constrain social interactions, and poor social interactions constrain motor skill development.

The results of both the cross-sectional analysis and the longitudinal analysis also indicate that fine motor skills are also delayed in children with ASD and the children fall further behind as they get older. It is not clear why children with ASD have poor fine motor skills, but the impact on early learning opportunities and daily living skills is not trivial (Jasmin, et al., 2009).

It is possible that tactile sensitivities and an overall lack of social imitation contribute to these fine motor delays; however, the relative contributions of social and imitative deficits to fine motor skills is far from clear. This is an area that requires further study and emphasis in early interventions for children with ASD.

  • Jasmin E, Couture M, McKinley P, Reid G, Fombonne E, Gisel E. Sensori-motor and daily living skills of preschool children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.2009;39(2):231–241.
  • See original article for other references  http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21610184

Flanagan JE, Landa R, Bhat A, Bauman M. Head lag in infants at risk for autism: a preliminary study. Am J Occup Ther. 2012 Sep-Oct;66(5):577-85. doi: 10.5014/ajot.2012.004192. PubMed PMID: 22917124.

OBJECTIVE.Poor postural control during pull-to-sit is a predictor of developmental disruption in cerebral palsy and preterm populations but has not been examined in infants at risk for autism. We examined the association between head lag during pull-to-sit at age 6 mo and autismrisk status. METHOD.High-risk participants were siblings of children with autism. We studied one sample of 40 high-risk infants prospectively from 6-36 mo and obtained diagnostic classifications of autism or no autism. We conducted a subsequent between-group comparison with a new sample of 20 high-risk and 21 low-risk infants. RESULTS.Head lag was significantly associated with autism spectrum disorder at 36 mo (p = .020) and was more frequently observed in high-risk than in low-risk infants (p = .018). CONCLUSION.Head lag with other alterations in early development may be associated with autism risk and may serve as an early indicator of neurodevelopmental disruption. Results have clinical implications for occupational therapists in early intervention practice.  Access article via Research Gate


Libertus K, Sheperd KA, Ross SW, Landa RJ. Limited fine motor and grasping skills in 6-month-old infants at high risk for autism. Child Dev. 2014 Nov-Dec;85(6):2218-31. Full text 

Atypical motor behaviors are common among children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). However, little is known about onset and functional implications of differences in early motor development among infants later diagnosed with ASD. Two prospective experiments were conducted to investigate motor skills among 6-month-olds at increased risk (high risk) for ASD (N1  = 129; N2  = 46). Infants were assessed using the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL) and during toy play. Across both experiments, high-risk infants exhibited less mature object manipulation in a highly structured (MSEL) context and reduced grasping activity in an unstructured (free-play) context than infants with no family history of ASD. Longitudinal assessments suggest that between 6 and 10 months, grasping activity increases in high-risk infants.


Purpura, G., Costanzo, V., Chericoni, N., Puopolo, M., Scattoni, M. L., Muratori, F., & Apicella, F. (2017). Bilateral Patterns of Repetitive Movements in 6- to 12-Month-Old Infants with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1168. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01168 Full text 

Aim: Some patterns of repetitive movements and their frequency have been proved to distinguish infants with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) from infants with Typical Development (TD) and Developmental Delay (DD) from 12 months of life on. The purpose of this study is to investigate if a specific repertoire of repetitive movements is present earlier in life, and if their higher rate and duration could differentiate infants with ASD from infants with DD and TD aged between 6 and 12 months.

Method: We conducted a retrospective analysis of video-clips taken from home videos to compare the frequency and the duration of Repetitive Movement Episodes (RMEs) in a sample of 30 children equally distributed among the three groups.

Results: Significantly higher total scores in bilateral RMEs with arms, hands, fingers, and lower limbs were found to distinguish ASD infants from both DD and TD infants, with a satisfactory diagnostic efficiency. No significant difference was found between the distributions of unilateral RMEs between ASD and DD/TD.

Interpretation: Results indicate the presence at this age of an ASD-specific pattern of bilateral repetitive movements. We hypothesize a continuum between this pattern and the lack of variability in finalized and communicative movements and gestures observed in children with ASD during the second year of life.


Serdarevic F, Ghassabian A, van Batenburg-Eddes T, White T, Blanken LME, Jaddoe VWV, Verhulst FC, Tiemeier H. Infant muscle tone and childhood autistic traits: A longitudinal study in the general population. Autism Res. 2017 May;10(5):757-768. doi: 10.1002/aur.1739. Epub 2017 Feb 9. PubMed PMID: 28181411; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5444969.

In a longitudinal population-based study of 2,905 children, we investigated if infants' neuromotor development was associated with autistictraits in childhood. Overall motor development and muscle tone were examined by trained research assistants with an adapted version of Touwen's Neurodevelopmental Examination between ages 2 and 5 months. Tone was assessed in several positions and items were scored as normal, low, or high tone. Parents rated their children's autistic traits with the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) and the Pervasive Developmental Problems (PDP) subscale of the Child Behavior Checklist at 6 years. We defined clinical PDP if scores were >98th percentile of the norm population. Diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) was clinically confirmed in 30 children. We observed a modest association between overall neuromotor development in infants and autistic traits. Low muscle tone in infancy predicted autistic traits measured by SRS (adjusted beta = 0.05, 95% CI for B: 0.00-0.02, P = 0.01), and PDP (adjusted beta = 0.08, 95% CI for B: 0.04-0.10, P < 0.001). Similar results emerged for the association of low muscle tone and clinical PDP (adjusted OR = 1.36, 95% CI: 1.08-1.72, P = 0.01) at age 6 years. Results remained unchanged if adjusted for child intelligence. There was no association between high muscle tone and SRS or PDP. Exclusion of children with ASD diagnosis did not change the association. This large study showed a prospective association of infant muscle tone with autistic traits in childhood. Our findings suggest that early detection of low muscle tone might be a gateway to improve early diagnosis of ASD. Autism Res 2017, 10: 757-768. © 2017